Friday, March 29, 2024

Cheaters are not just human-Prairie Voles are too

Prairie voles, a common type of Midwestern rodent, are typically one of the few monogamous mammals in the world, living with long-term partners in underground nests and raising litters of pups together. Pictured above is a bank vole. (Credit: Hanna Knutsson/Flickr)
Prairie voles, a common type of Midwestern rodent, are typically one of the few monogamous mammals in the world, living with long-term partners in underground nests and raising litters of pups together. Pictured above is a bank vole. (Credit: Hanna Knutsson/Flickr)

Animals may at times behave differently from their peers in social contexts, but scientists can rarely pinpoint why the outliers act the way they do. A new study sheds light on that question, through the case of the philandering prairie vole.

Male prairie voles with poor spatial memory are more likely to cheat on their mates—and both environmental factors and genetic predispositions explain why. Faithful and unfaithful males have different genetics that translate into different patterns of space use, and these behavioral differences lead to each male type breeding successfully under different population densities.

“IT’S A TRADE-OFF. IF YOU ARE CHEATING, YOU ARE MORE LIKELY TO BE CHEATED ON.”

“What we’re really getting at in this paper is individual variation in genes and gene expression is coding for individual differences in behavior,” says coauthor Alexander Ophir, assistant professor of psychology at Cornell University. “This is an amazing example of how you can follow the big-scale ecological behavior down to the molecular underpinnings and then back up again.

“Evolutionary pressures are influencing the maintenance of these different genetic factors that ultimately control the expression of this gene and then translate into the behaviors that you see.”

Scientists have long known that prairie voles, a common type of Midwestern rodent, are typically one of the few monogamous mammals in the world, living with long-term partners in underground nests and raising litters of pups together. They have also known that some males tend to roam into neighboring territory and mate with unguarded females, and that these cheating males have genes that are expressed differently in the part of the brain that relates to spatial memory.

The new study published in Science shows that these genes can be influenced by such environmental factors as population density, and that the cheating voles have more reproductive success when the population is high, while faithful prairie voles do better reproductively when the population is low.

“If the population density is high, the chances of getting cheated on goes up, because there are a lot of other males around. So it’s that high population density that favors being an unfaithful ‘married’ prairie vole,” Ophir says.

“But when the population crashes, then those chance encounters should be more infrequent, and guarding your mate should be more effective. It’s a trade-off. If you are cheating, you are more likely to be cheated on. So you stay and protect or take your chances and cheat.”

Spatial memory comes into play because being monogamous is a spatial task, Ophir says. “You need to know who your neighbors are and where they are. Some are competitors, some are potential mates,” Ophir says.

A male with good spatial memory will stay at home and ensure his mate only has his children. But if a wandering male can’t remember being chased off by a competitor, he may return to find an opportunity to mate. “Both strategies are beneficial, depending on the population density.

Researchers from the University of Texas at Austin are coauthors of the work.

Source: Cornell University and Futurity.org

LEAVE A REPLY

Please enter your comment!
Please enter your name here

online wholesale business for goods from
China